Pressed into the quiet world of the womb, fetuses can prepare to howl.

In the same way, newborns can cry as soon as they are born, ordinary marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) make contact calls for attention from those caring for them. These vocalizations are not improvisation, the researchers said in a pre-press release published April 14 in bioRxiv. Ultrasound image of marmoset fetuses reveals that their mouths are already imitating the distinctive pattern of movements used to make their first calls, long before the production of sound.

Early behaviors in babies are usually described as “congenital” or “firmly connected,” but a team at Princeton University wondered exactly how that behavior developed. How does a baby know how to cry as soon as it is born?

The secret may lie in what happens before birth. “Humans tend to ignore the fetal period,” said Darshana Narayanan, a behavioral neurologist who did the research while at Princeton University. “They just think the baby is just vegetating and waiting to be born.” [But] that’s where a lot of things start. “

Research shows, for example, that chickens inside their eggs are already learning to identify the call of their species (SN: 16.9.21). “It’s developing so much earlier in development than we thought before,” said developmental psychobiologist Samantha Caruso-Peck, executive director of the Grassland Bird Trust in Fort Edward, New York, who was not involved in the study. But, she says, “we haven’t really seen much of it from the production side.” Most of what we know is the auditory side. “Caruso-Peck studies vocal training in songbirds and how it applies to how people learn language.

Narayanan and his colleagues turned to marmosets because the development of vocalization in monkeys is similar to that in humans (SN: 13.8.15). Teams of two people performed non-invasive ultrasounds on two marmosets almost every day during four different pregnancies. The effort involved a lot of marshmallow fluff, Narayanan said. “They would do anything for marshmallows.”

About 95 days after pregnancy, the face of the fetus appears for the first time. Researchers have noticed that each young fetus moves its mouth and other parts of its face in tandem with its head. As the pregnancy progressed, facial and head features began to move independently. Separating these different motor regions probably prepares the fetus for tasks such as nutrition or vocalization.

Ultrasound of a marmoset in the womb shows that the movements of the fetus’ face and mouth (left) are similar to a baby’s contact call (right), suggesting that the fetus develops motor skills to cry before birth and can make sound. .

It was a matter of lip-reading then. Did the fetus just move its mouth, or did the movements mimic certain sounds? It was clear to researchers at the beginning that the movements of the mouth are similar to the movements that the marmoset makes during contact, characterized by its long duration and many syllables.

“The contact call is so unique that you really can’t mistake it for any of the other calls,” said Princeton biologist Asif Gazanfar.

But to confirm, Gazanfar, Narayanan and their team tracked the movements of the fetal jaw frame by frame to determine their duration. They also measure the number of “syllables” that the fetus makes by calculating the number of movements separated by less than 500 milliseconds. The researchers then compared the movements of the fetus with the contact calls caused by the young marmosets after birth. As the fetus approaches birth, the movements of the face and mouth become more similar to those of the baby’s contact calls – evidence, the team says, that the fetus develops the ability to make this call after birth.

This supports the idea that early cries do not “appear magically,” Narayanan said. “They have a long period of development – but in utero.”

Baby marmosets may practice their first distinctive cries in the womb

Previous articleBitcoin is falling again, now in the middle of its highest November
Next articleA revolutionary new Qubit platform can transform quantum computing